The Limbic Region "Emotion Central" Website | home
General Psychology | Poetry of the Afflicted & Survivors | Art by the Afflicted & Survivors | Mental Innuendos & More! | images
General Psychology
Emotion
Dr. C. George Boeree
So far, our theory is rather cold and mechanical. What about
feelings? Well, they're there, to some degree, in every
interaction. Imagine this: In the middle of the night, you get a bad
case of the mad munchies. So you leave your bed and head for the
fridge. It's very dark, but you know your apartment like the back
of your hand, so you don't bother with the lights. The coffee
table is in the middle of the room and you anticipate its presence
and maneuver around it. Perhaps you reach out your hand to
touch the edge to confirm your anticipation. You're almost there
-- five more feet to the fridge -- when WHAM! you walk into a solid
six foot...something: The unanticipated! What do you feel at that
moment? Perhaps fear, surprise, perhaps sheer terror. Whatever
it is, it is rather unpleasant. Let's call it distress. You are, at the
same time, busy "generating anticipations" -- making guesses about
the nature of the beast, taking actions that might alleviate some
of your fears, dashing for the light switch. The lights come on...
you're expecting a sex-crazed psycho-killer.... And lo and behold,
it's the fridge. You cleaned behind it for the first time in 30 years
and left it pulled out. Now how do you feel? Perhaps you feel
relief, a sensation of pleasant resolution. You heave a great
sigh, perhaps laugh. Things make sense again. Life is on the right
path again. Let's call it delight. (Note that you might still feel
some negative emotion as well, as soon as the initial relief is
behind you -- like annoyance at your own stupidity. That problem
has yet to be resolved!) Another example: Notice the people
coming off one of the "sooper-dooper" roller coasters. Notice
their frozen smiles. That's their way of saying "yes! I am alive!"
Let's be more precise: When interaction is problematic, we feel
distress. For example, (1) when we fail to anticipate something--
like the fridge in our face--we are distressed. We also feel
distress when (2) we anticipate more than one thing at the same
time: conflicting anticipations. Which of your roommates is
actually the chain-saw killer? Each time you are alone with one
of them, you don't know whether to feel secure or to run like
the blazes. And (3) we also feel it when we are faced by general
uncertainty: Which way is that cockroach, or rat, or snake going
to move next? Perhaps this is the root of our common phobias of
these delightful creatures. Distress can be mild, an irritation or
annoyance: When your pen runs out of ink just as you sign a
check at the local supermarket. It can be a bit more intense: The
frustration of you car breaking down; the fear as your car
careens out of control on the highway; the disgust you feel
when you discover that your lover bites the heads off of live
chickens. Delight is the resolution of our distressful problems.
We are, actually, developing or elaborating our understanding
of the world when we feel delight. Delight is the emotional side
of adaptation, of (believe it or not!) learning. It too can be mild:
The pleasant feeling of finishing a crossword puzzle or winning
at a game or sport. Or it can be a bit more intense, like the relief
you feel when you realize that the roller-coaster only felt like
it was leaving the tracks; or the joy of scientific discovery,
artistic creation, or mystical experience. Notice that since
solving problems requires having problems, delight depends on
distress. Even physical pleasure seems to work like this: You
enjoy it more after doing without it for a while, whether "it" is
food, drink, or sex! Too much of it, and it doesn't seem to satisfy
quite so well. (Note that our response to this is often to try
doing it even more! Hence some of our neurotic attitudes
towards sex, food, gambling, attention....) Facing a problem
doesn't cause distress -- it is distress. The distress is just the
feeling-side of the situation. The same points apply to delight. It
isn't caused by problem-resolution, it is problem-resolution. And
distress and delight don't cause you to seek a solution; they are
not "motivating forces." But there's no doubt that the situations
in which you feel distress may be ones that you avoid in the future.
Or, if they resulted in delight, they may be ones you seek out in
the future. It is the anticipation of distress or delight that is
motivating. Anxiety is the distressful anticipation of distress.
From experience, you expect that the situation before you will be
unpleasant. This expectation is itself unpleasant: it conflicts
with your desire to be a happy, carefree individual. And, often, you
try to avoid the situation. Hope is the delightful anticipation of
delight. From experience, the problem before you will be
resolved, and this is a happy thought. Depending on details, we
could also call this eagerness, or even anxiety, as in "I'm anxious
to get started!" Now, the "basic" distress and delight don't
usually happen at the same time--since one is the problem and the
other the solution. But anticipatory distress and delight -- that
is, anxiety and hope -- often happen at the same time: We call this
"mixed emotions." Skimming across deep water on little sticks at
30 miles per hour can make you nervous; water-skiing, on the
other hand, sounds like fun. You feel both anxiety and eagerness.
You decision whether to try it will be based on how these two
balance out for you. Notice I said "for you." The decision is very
much a subjective one, based on what makes you anxious and
eager. Anticipation can also help us make sense of other
emotions, such as anger: Anger is distress with an expectation of
external change. The problem is "out there" and anger is the
build-up of energy needed to solve it. Just try to hold back a baby
from crawling, and see what you get. Sadness is distress with an
expectation of internal change. The problem is "in here." I realize
that I must adapt to it. Grief is the most obvious example: You
can't get them back; you can only learn to live with their
absence. Many of our major learning experiences involve
sadness, such as coming to understand our own limitations, or
the limitations of our loved ones, for example. Notice that anger
is a little more hopeful; sadness is a little harder to take. People
tend to be angry at things before they settle down to accept
what they can't change. That says something very important
about us: We resist major changes in the self; if we can, we try to
make the world fit our expectations. Sometimes people persist in
these emotional states. A person who is always trying to make the
world -- especially others -- fit his expectations we call
aggressive, and his emotional state hostile.. Often, what he
really needs to do is change himself, adapt. But for some reason -
- his culture, for example -- giving-in is taboo. Like physical
pleasures, when it doesn't work right, we do what we always do,
only more! Likewise, a person who is always trying to make himself
fit the world -- and especially others' expectations -- we call
compliant and his emotional state is commonly depressed.. He is
always trying to adjust himself to others, when often what he
needs is to get angry. Most common of all is avoidance: When we
see a problem coming, we give in to our anxiety and run away,
physically or psychologically. With avoidance, we are really
trying to get out of an emotional situation and back into a
peaceful state. Unfortunately, if you avoid problems and their
distress, you also avoid the delight of solutions. Think of some
of the common "psychological" ways we avoid life's problems:
Alcohol, drugs, television. The goal of avoidance is to be
unconscious, or at least unconscious of problems. These three
"types" -- aggressive, compliant, and avoiding -- are so common that
a number of theorists have independently come up with them
(Adler, Horney, Fromm, and others). These types may even have a
genetic component to them, so that some of us are more likely to
deal with our problems by turning to aggression, others with
compliance, still others with avoidance. More mature people
tend to take on problems with an eye towards a solution: They
face distress and anxiety with hope and eagerness. This takes a
little something--an ability to focus on your goals, and to ignore
the pains of getting there. This has been called will-power, self-
discipline, need for achievement, delay-of-gratification, and
emotional intelligence. I just call it will.
Theories about emotions It has always been assumed that the
First thing that happens is that we experience an emotion, and
then and only then do we start reacting to the situation
physiologically. But over a hundred years ago, William James, the
father of American psychology, and Carl Lange, a Danish
psychologist, separately introduced the idea that we have it all
backwards: First, they said, we have physiological responses to a
situation, and only then do we use those responses to formulate
an experience of emotion. This is called the James-Lange theory.
Walter Cannon and Phillip Bard came up with a variation on the
James-Lange idea in 1929: They suggested that there are neural
paths from our senses that go in two directions. One goes to
the cortex, where we have a subjective experience, and one goes
to the hypothalamus, where the physiological processes begin.
In other words, the experience of an emotion, and the
physiological responses occur together. This is (as you might
expect by now) called the Cannon-Bard theory. In 1937, James
Papez noted that the physiological side of emotion is not just a
matter of the hypothalamus, but is a complex network of neural
pathways -- the Papez circuit. In 1949, Paul McLean completed and
corrected Papez’s ideas, and called the larger complex the
limbic system, which is what we call it today. It included the
hypothalamus, the hippocampus, and the amygdala, and is tightly
connected with the cingulate gyrus, the ventral tegmental area
of the brain stem, the septum, and the prefrontal gyrus. Paul
McLean is also the founder of the triune brain theory. He
suggested that there is a certain evolutionary quality to the
structure of the brain. Reptiles, he said, function entirely in
terms of instinct, and their brains are little more than what we
called the brain stem in people. He called it the archipallium or
reptilian brain, and it includes the medulla, cerebellum, the pons,
and the olfactory bulbs. Above this is the paleopallium, or old
mammalian brain. This is the limbic system and the portions of the
brain we call the old cortex. Of course, this adds emotions to
the reptilian picture, and allows for simple conditioning. And on
top of the paleopallium is the neopallium (aka new mammalian or
rational brain, or neocortex). This is where more advanced
activities occur, including awareness. McLean adds that, in
human beings, these three “brains” don’t always behave
cooperatively, which leads to some of the unique problems we
have!
Basic emotions One question that is asked repeatedly is “what are
the basic emotions.” There have been dozens of answers to this,
none of which have been completely satisfying. This is, no doubt,
due to the fact that emotional response is complex to begin with,
and is made even more complex by the fact that we add our
thoughts and interpretations to them as well as just
“experiencing” them as they are. I suggest that we can organize
emotions into seven families:
The Surprise Family
surprise, startle, astonishment
bewilderment, confusion
shock, exhaustion, overwhelmedness
The Fear Family
fear, threat, terror
anxiety
doubt, caution, suspicion
The Anger Family
anger, rage, frustration
hatred, hostility
envy, jealousy
disgust, contempt, annoyance, indignation
smugness, self-satisfaction, schadenfreud
The Sadness Family
sadness, sorrow, depression
anguish, despair
grief, loneliness
shame, embarrassment, humiliation
guilt, remorse, regret
The Eagerness Family
eagerness, anticipation, excitement, confidence
hopefulness
curiosity, interest
The Happiness Family
happiness, elation, joy, gladness
contentment, satisfaction
self-satisfaction, pride
love, affection, compassion
amusement, humor, laughter
The Boredom Family
boredom, ennui, complacency
©Copyright 2002, C. George Boeree
(Green)Archipallium brain (reptilian brain)
(Red)Palleomammalian brain (limbic system)
(Blue)Neopallium brain (neocortex)
Support The Limbic Region
Now!
|
||